PS: Political Science &
Politics, June 1998 v31 n2 p182(8)
Monica Lewinsky's contribution to political
science. John R. Zaller.
Abstract: The increase in Bill Clinton's popularity
ratings when the controversy with Monica Lewinsky was tempered
by reports of an economic boom proves that American politics
is driven more by political substance rather than by
media-generated politics. It also proves that the media's
preoccupation with exposing presidential gaffes negatively
affect public support but only briefly. Only if media politics
conspires with the partisan opposition can there be a profound
political effect.
Full Text: COPYRIGHT 1998 American Political Science
Association
The bounce in President Clinton's job ratings that occurred
in the initial 10 days of the Lewinsky imbroglio may offer as
much insight into the dynamics of public opinion as any single
event in recent memory. What it shows is not just the power of
a booming economy to buttress presidential popularity. It
shows, more generally, the importance of political substance,
as against media hype, in American politics. Even when, as
occurred in this case, public opinion is initially responsive
to media reports of scandal, the public's concern with actual
political achievement reasserts itself. This lesson, which was
not nearly so clear before the Lewinsky matter as it is now,
not only deepens our understanding of American politics. It
also tends, as I argue in the second half of this article, to
undermine the importance of one large branch of public opinion
research, buttress the importance of another, and point toward
some new research questions.
Whatever else may have transpired by the time this article
gets into print, the Lewinsky poll bounce is something worth
pondering. In a half-dozen commercial polls taken in the
period just before the story broke, Clinton's job approval
rating averaged about 60%. Ten days later, following intensive
coverage of the story and Clinton's State of the Union
address, presidential support was about 10 percentage points
higher.(1) The fact that no analyst of public opinion could
have credibly predicted this outcome makes the poll bounce
especially important to examine. It is, in statistical
parlance, a high leverage case.
I begin my analysis with an attempt to establish the
parameters of the initial public response to the Lewinsky
matter. Toward this end, the results of some three dozen
commercial polls, gleaned from published sources, are
summarized in Table 1. Although question wordings differ
somewhat, all poll results refer to approval of Clinton's job
performance as president. Also reported in Table 1 are the
results of a content analysis of network TV news coverage
during this period.
The content analysis, as shown in the top three rows of the
table, gives average minutes of each network news program that
were favorable or unfavorable to Clinton. Favorable references
include Clinton's denials, attacks on Independent Prosecutor
Ken Starr, statements of support for Clinton, and any other
information (including non-scandal information) that might
tend to enhance public support for the president. Unfavorable
references include all statements indicating that the
president had an affair with Lewinsky or tried to cover it up,
attacks on Clinton or [TABULAR DATA FOR TABLE 1 OMITTED]
defense of Starr, and any other information that might tend to
undermine public support for Clinton. I emphasize that,
although journalists played a major role in creating the
Lewinsky imbroglio, other actors, notably politicians,
initiated some of the information that was reported.
What the content analysis shows is that the frenzy began
with two days of heavily negative coverage, but that coverage
was relatively balanced after that (given that the matter
continued to attract media attention at all). In fact, if the
first two days are removed, the remaining period has about as
many positive minutes as negative ones, including two days on
which Clinton's coverage was decidedly positive.
I have divided the poll data into four partially
overlapping periods. As the table indicates, the first two
days of heavily negative scandal reportage had a considerable
impact on public opinion. On the basis of a half-dozen polls,
Clinton's public support seems to have dropped about six or
seven points.(2)
The scandal broke on a Wednesday, with the most heavily
negative coverage on that day and Thursday. From Friday on,
coverage was more balanced and public support for the
president rose. By Monday, Clinton had regained everything
lost in the first two days, and in Tuesday's Gallup poll,
support for the President rose above pre-Lewinsky levels.
There were two notable events in this period, both of which
were amply reported on TV news. The first was Clinton's
appearance on camera on Monday to make an emotional denial of
a sexual relationship with Lewinsky; the other was Hillary
Clinton's appearance on NBC's Today Show on Tuesday morning,
where she charged the existence of a right-wing conspiracy
against her husband.
If there is any particular spike in the data, it is the
Tuesday Gallup poll, which was taken between six and nine in
the evening and was therefore able to reflect news of Hillary
Clinton's appearance on the Today Show that morning. Indeed,
the poll was taken just as or just after many Americans were
getting news of Mrs. Clinton's appearance; it may therefore,
as other polls hint, have overstated its lasting importance on
opinion. This poll showed a gain of eight percentage points
from the day before, a difference that is statistically
significant on a two-tailed test.(3)
Clinton's State of the Union address occurred on Tuesday
evening, the end of the seventh day since the Lewinsky story
broke. The speech attracted an unusually large audience,
presumably because people wanted to see how the
crisis-stricken President would perform. According to
virtually all the pundits, he performed extremely well. "Good
speech, too bad," as one commentator put it.(4)
Two national surveys were taken immediately after the
speech. From baselines on the day before the speech - and
therefore before Hillary Clinton's charge of right-wing
conspiracy - one survey showed no change and the other showed
a gain of five points, for an average gain of 2.5%.(5) There
was also a CBS poll involving reinterviews with a panel of
respondents who had been asked by telephone to watch the
speech so that they could be polled afterward. This survey
found that Clinton's post-speech job approval rating was 73%.
No immediate prespeech baseline for this poll is available,
but if we take the best baseline we have - Clinton's 57% job
approval in the CBS-New York Times poll from the day before
the speech - then the combination of the speech and Mrs.
Clinton's defense netted the president some 16 percentage
points in support.
A little back-of-the-envelope arithmetic shows that these
two sets of post-speech results - an average 2.5% gain in two
polls and a 16-point gain among those asked to watch the
speech - are not as far apart as they might seem. According to
the Nielsen research firm, 53.1 million Americans saw the
speech ("TV Ratings for Speech," 1998). This is a lot of
people, but only about 25% of the adult population. If 16% of
the 25% who watched the speech became more supportive of the
president, the overall increase in public support would be
only 4.0 percentage points (.16 x .25 = .04). If we assume
that viewership of the speech was higher than 25% among those
asked to watch it in preparation for a survey but still well
under 100%, there is no real disagreement among the three
polls on the size of the "speech plus Hillary" effect.
From the bottom panel of Table 1, it appears that public
support for Clinton rose another three or so points after the
State of the Union, perhaps in response to favorable news
coverage of that event. But this gain, if real, is apparently
small in relation to gains that had already occurred.
It is tempting to pursue more detailed analyses of
particular events, but I have already pressed dangerously
close to the limits of the data. Instead, I will step back and
offer a somewhat less detailed and, I therefore hope, safer
summary: In response to sharply negative media coverage of the
Lewinsky matter, public support for the president fell. But
support rebounded and then surpassed its initial level as the
president, his wife, and their allies fought back.
One point seems especially clear and important: In the
period in which Clinton's support fell about 7 percentage
points, media coverage was sharply negative, but in the period
in which he gained back those 7 points and added an additional
8 to 10 points of support, coverage was essentially balanced.
Thus, while media coverage of the Lewinsky matter explains
part of the opinion change that occurred, it cannot explain
all of it. In particular, the notion that the public responded
mechanically to media coverage cannot explain how Clinton
ended up with higher job approval ratings than he began with.
Additional explanation is needed.
An obvious possibility is to argue that the public makes a
distinction between approving the way the president does his
job and approving of the president as a person. There is, as
it happens, some evidence for this view, but not a great deal.
The president's personal favorability ratings fell more
sharply than his job approval ratings and also recovered less
well. In three NBC News polls, Clinton's favorability ratings
were 57% before the Lewinsky matter broke, 40% after three
days of scandal coverage, and 50% after the State of the
Union. In what is apparently the only other set of surveys
that made three such soundings of opinion, Time-CNN found that
Clinton's favorability ratings went from 60% to 50% and then
back to 60%.(6)
These data on favorability seem to me to do little to
alleviate the mystery of Clinton's bounce in job approval
ratings, since they show essentially the same trend. Even if
we were examining the favorability data alone, we would still
be hard-pressed to explain why Clinton, who looked nothing
like a teflon president when he was pressing for gays in the
military and health reform, stood up to the scandal coverage
as well as he did. Nor could we explain why, amidst continued
media attention to scandal, he actually recouped most of his
initial loss.
Another argument might be that Clinton's specific defense
against the allegations of sex and cover-up was simply very
persuasive. But I find this hard to swallow - not because I
disbelieve Clinton, but because he presented so little
evidence to support his side and got so little support from
witnesses that were in a position to give it. In particular,
Clinton got no help from Lewinsky herself, who was
semi-publicly negotiating a plea bargain with the independent
prosecutor throughout this period. As I parse Clinton's
defense, it has consisted of two flat assertions: "I didn't do
it" and "my enemies are out to get me."
If the public believed this defense, it was because it
wanted to. I suggest, therefore, that we consider the
political context that presumably made the public want to
believe Clinton's defense, namely, his record of achievement
in office. Clinton made an excellent statement of this record
in his State of the Union address. Although the address
reached too few people and came too late to explain the bulk
of Clinton's recovery in the polls, it is reasonable to
suppose that the presidential record that the speech touted
was well-known to the majority of the public.
Clinton speech was, first of all, a celebration of a list
of "accomplishments" that would be any president's dream: The
economy was the strongest in 25 years, the federal budget was
on the verge of balance for the first time in 20 years, crime
was falling for the first time in living memory, and the
country was at peace. In the main section of the speech, the
president proposed a series of programs designed to appeal to
the ideological center, as exemplified by a plan to use
surplus funds to put Social Security on a sound footing,
improve public education, and build more highways. Thus, what
the president trumpeted in his speech - and what he would
presumably continue by remaining in office - was a record of
peace, prosperity, and moderation. Or, more succinctly, it was
a record of "political substance." This record was so
unassailable that, to much of what the president said in the
State of the Union, the Republican leadership could only offer
polite applause.
TABLE 2
The Effect of Peace, Prosperity, and Moderation on Presidential
Vote, 1948-1996
Two-sided
B S.E. p-value
War -4.5 2.3 .04
(52, 68 = 1, else = 0)
Real Disposable Income(a) 2.1 0.40 .001
(range: 0% to 7.7%)
Relative Extremism -3.3 1.0 .005
(see text)
Constant 43.6
Adjusted r-square .77
N = 13
Note: Dependent variable is percentage of the two-party vote for the
incumbent party candidate.
a From Survey of Current Business, August, 1997, Table 4, p. 164-67.
Can political substance, thus defined, move public opinion?
Certainly it can. Thanks to a distinguished series of studies
- including Key (1966), Kramer (1971), Mueller (1971), Fiorina
(1981), and Rosenstone (1983) - political scientists have been
aware of the importance of "bottom line" politics for some
time. Brody's (1991) work on presidential popularity, which
stresses the effects of "outcomes" news coverage on approval,
points in the same direction. In light of this, it seems
entirely plausible to suggest that the poll bounce that
Clinton got at the time of the Lewinsky matter was driven by
the same thing that drives presidential election outcomes and
presidential popularity in general - political substance. It
was not admiration for Bill Clinton's character that first
buttressed and then boosted his approval ratings. It was the
public's reaction to the delivery of outcomes and policies
that the public wants.
This argument is much more than a claim that "It's the
economy, stupid." In fact, Clinton's economic performance has
been only middling through most of his presidency. Taking the
average four-year growth in Real Disposable Income (RDI) for
every president elected from 1948 on, Clinton's first term
economy ranks tenth of 13. If presidential terms are
rank-ordered by growth in the 12-month period prior to
Election Day, Clinton's first term is still a mediocre tenth
of 13 since World War II. Only recently has Clinton's economic
performance become as strong as he described it in his State
of the Union.
If Clinton's economy cannot by itself explain why he won by
nine percentage points over Bob Dole in 1996, neither can it
explain trends in his approval ratings. One big but
easy-to-overlook factor is peace, which is a virtual
prerequisite for popular support. Popular support for
Presidents Truman and Johnson was so damaged by bloody wars
that, despite reasonably good economies, they chose not to run
for reelection.(7) Clinton's administration has not only
avoided war, it has enjoyed a very notable success in Bosnia,
for which the President was nominated for a Nobel Peace Prize.
The other big and also easy-to-overlook plus for Clinton is
his ideological moderation. This is a factor that scholars,
with the exceptions of Rosenstone (1983) and Alesina, et al.
(1993), have too often ignored. Let me first show anecdotally
how moderation affected Clinton's support and then, insofar as
possible, make a systematic case.
Since gays in the military and the debacle of health care
reform, Clinton has hewed to centrist policies, including
ones, like welfare reform (and NAFTA earlier on), that are
hard for Democratic presidents to endorse. In his
confrontation with the Republican Congress over balancing the
budget, it was the president, rather than the Republicans, who
held middle ground. And finally, after two decades of massive
budget deficits, the president has, by means of an initially
unpopular budget package in his first term, helped bring the
centrist goal of a balanced budget within apparent grasp.
Consistent with the notion that moderation matters is this
fragment of hard evidence: President Clinton's approval
ratings were weaker at the midpoint of his first term, when
the economy was stronger but he identified himself with
noncentrist policies, than at the end of his first term, when
the economy was weaker but he had remade himself as a policy
moderate. Clinton's average job approval rating in Gallup
polls taken in the sixth, seventh, and eighth quarters
averaged 44.3% and the average percent change in RDI in these
same quarters was 4.7%. In quarters fourteen through sixteen,
these figures were 55.5% and 1.5%.
Systematic evidence that policy moderation affects
presidential popularity is, as far as I know, non-existent.
But as regards presidential elections, the evidence, though
limited, is clear. The only published evidence comes from
Rosenstone's Forecasting Presidential Elections (1983), which
finds centrism to be a major determinant of cross-state and
cross-time voting. In another cut at this problem, my research
assistant rated each of the candidates in elections from 1948
to 1996 on a seven-point scale, running from liberal (+3) to
conservative (-3). The ratings of each pair of candidates were
then summed to produce a measure of relative distance from the
center - i.e., a measure of relative extremism - such that
higher scores indicated greater relative distance from the
midpoint by the candidate of the incumbent party. For example,
Lyndon Johnson was rated +2 in 1964 and Barry Goldwater as -3,
so that Goldwater was one point further from the center than
Johnson. Obviously, such ratings are subject to error and
bias. But I note that they were developed in connection with
another project (press bias in presidential primaries), and
that they correlate highly with the ratings of Rosenstone with
which they overlap. These ratings also correlate well with a
new set of ideological location scores produced by Poole
(forthcoming) for presidential candidates who earlier served
in Congress.(8)
The results for a standard voting model are shown in Table
2. War is coded as "1" in 1952 and 1968 and "0" otherwise.
Economic performance is measured as average percent change in
RDI in the four quarters prior to the election; that is, in
the 12th through 15th quarters of each term. As examination of
the regression coefficients in Table 2 shows, ideological
extremism rivals economic performance as a determinant of vote
for the incumbent party. Being one point closer to the center
on a seven-point ideology scale (as Johnson was in 1964) is
worth about 3 percentage points of the vote; by way of
comparison, each additional percent of growth in RDI is worth
about 2.1 percentage points. Finally, war costs the incumbent
party about 4.5 percentage points of the vote.(9)
From all this I conclude that peace, prosperity, and
moderation very heavily influence the dynamics of presidential
support, probably in matters of presidential popularity and
certainly in general elections, for Clinton as well as for
other presidents. What the Lewinsky bounce adds to this
conclusion is confidence. Although evidence of the importance
of political substance has been accumulating for some three
decades, no one could have predicted that Clinton would
survive the opening round of the Lewinsky affair nearly so
well as he did. This is because it has never been quite so
starkly clear just how relentlessly the majority of voters can
stay focused on the bottom line. Nor, to my knowledge, has it
ever been quite so clear that it is possible for public
opinion and media opinion to go marching off in opposing
directions.
To argue, as I am, that the public stays focused on a
bottom line consisting of peace, prosperity, and moderation is
not to say that the public is either wise or virtuous. For one
thing, its sense of substance seems, in the aggregate, rather
amoral - usually more like "what have you done for me lately"
than "social justice." Nor is it clear that its decision
criteria are very sophisticated. Suppose, for example, that
the Watergate investigation of Richard Nixon had taken place
in the context of Bill Clinton's booming economy rather than,
as was the case, in the context of gasoline shortages and
"stagflation" (the combination of high inflation and high
unemployment). Would Nixon have been forced from office under
these circumstances? Or, if Clinton were saddled with Nixon's
economy, would Clinton be, at this point, on the verge of
impeachment? These are, I believe, real questions, and the
fact that they are does not speak well for the public's wisdom
or virtue.
Perhaps future events will shed clearer light on these
questions. From the vantage point of early April, when this
essay is being finalized, I am keenly aware that issues
relating to Lewinsky, Whitewater, and Paula Jones have by no
means reached a conclusion. If clear evidence of sexual
harassment, perjury, or obstruction of justice emerges, the
public might still turn on Clinton. If so, one's judgment of
public opinion would need to be more favorable: It waits for
clear evidence before reaching a verdict, and it is, after
all, concerned with higher values. My personal hunch, however,
is that public support for Clinton will be more affected by
future performance of the economy than by the clarity of the
evidence concerning the charges against him.
I said in opening this article, the Lewinsky affair
buttresses some work in political science and undermines the
importance of other work. The tradition of studies on economic
and retrospective voting, which maintains that the public
responds to the substance of party performance, seems
strengthened by the Lewinsky matter. On the other hand, the
tradition of studies that focuses on the mass media, political
psychology, and elite influence, including such diverse
studies as Edelman's Symbolic Uses of Politics (1964) and my
own Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion (1992), seems somewhat
weaker. It is reasonable to contend that the ground has
shifted beneath these two traditions in a way that scholars
will need to accommodate. However poorly informed,
psychologically driven, and "mass mediated" public opinion may
be, it is capable of recognizing and focusing on its own
conception of what matters. This is not a conclusion that
comes naturally to the second tradition.
Let me amplify the nature of the aspersion I have just
cast. A major development in American politics in the last 50
to 100 years has been the rise of what has been variously
called The Rhetorical Presidency (Tulis 1987), the "political
spectacle" (Edelman 1988) and, more simply, Media Politics.
This form of politics stands in contrast to an older model of
politics, Party Politics. The defining feature of what I
prefer to call Media Politics is the attempt to govern on the
basis of words and images that diffuse through the mass media.
This communication - whether in the form of presidential
speeches, press conferences, TV ads, media frenzies, spin, or
ordinary news - creates a sort of virtual reality whose
effects are arguably quite real and important. Typical of the
attitude that prevails in this style of politics is Republican
strategist Frank Luntz's assessment of the events I have just
analyzed: "The problem with [the Lewinsky matter] is we are
not going to learn the real impact for years. . . . It's going
to leave an indelible mark on our psyche but I don't know what
the mark will be. . ." (quoted in Connolly and Edsall 1998).
Freely translated, what Luntz is saying is: "It may take us in
the spin business a little time to figure out how to play
this, but you can be sure we'll keep it alive until we come up
with something that works for our side."
As a Republican strategist, Luntz has an obvious partisan
interest in taking this view. But his occupational interest is
equally great. He and his colleagues in both parties have an
interest in "constructing" a public discourse in which events
like the Lewinsky affair are important and in which political
substance - in the sense of peace, prosperity, and moderation
- is unimportant, except insofar as it is useful to emphasize
it.
A sizeable part of political science has organized itself
to study this new political style. My analysis of the Lewinsky
affair, however, suggests that political science not go too
far down this road, since old-fashioned political substance of
the kind that party competition brings to the fore is not only
thriving in the media age, but quite likely still dominant.
This is not to say that the new style of Media Politics is
without importance. If only for the resources it consumes and
the public attention it commands, Media Politics matters.
More, perhaps, than we would like, Media Politics defines our
political culture. But beyond that, the effects of Media
Politics on political outcomes must be demonstrated on a
case-by-case basis, because sometimes the effects are real and
lasting and other times they are not.
One illuminating example of Media Politics that produced
lasting effects is Gerald Ford's pardon of Richard Nixon in
1974. Coverage of the event was, of course, overwhelmingly
negative. On the basis of the same coding categories as in
Table 1, Ford got 11 minutes of negative coverage on the
network news on the night following the pardon, as against two
minutes of positive coverage. The next night, these figures
were 10 and 2 minutes. Reporters were by no means the only
source of the bad news. In the first two news days after the
pardon, 12 Democratic members of Congress, including the House
Speaker and Senate Majority Leader, were quoted on the network
news attacking Ford, and within the first week the Democratic
Congress passed a resolution condemning the pardon. Three
Republican leaders also criticized Ford. In these
circumstances, Ford's approval rating fell 17 percentage
points in the first two days and about 30 points over the
longer run.
The contrast with the Lewinsky case is striking. In the
first two days of this case, only three Republican members of
Congress, none from the leadership, were willing to be quoted
on network TV news attacking Clinton - and not for want of
opportunity. Reporters were scouring Capitol Hill for
volunteers, but politicians (including Democratic politicians)
were playing it safe. Thus, the media were forced to shoulder
a much larger part of the Lewinsky story on their own. In
these quite different circumstances, Clinton suffered limited
short-term damage and made gains over the longer run.
It is a tempting conclusion that when the partisan
opposition joins a media frenzy, the two together can move
public opinion, but that the media alone cannot do it. But
even if systematic research were to establish that this
pattern is general, there would still be an obvious concern:
Namely, that opposition politicians attack when they see an
opportunity to score points and hold fire otherwise. By this
account, Democratic politicians attacked Ford because they
knew the attacks would play well, but Republican politicians
refrained from attacking Clinton because they feared the
attacks would backfire. If this argument is considered
plausible, as I think it must be, it further underscores the
central claim of this essay: That American politics tends to
be driven more by political substance - in this case, public
disapproval of the pardon of Nixon - than by the antics of
Media Politics. It also points to a difficult future research
problem: Sorting out whether partisan attacks and other media
messages are the causes of public attitudes or their hidden
(i.e., endogenous) effects. Surely, the answer is some of
both.
Another media frenzy from the Ford administration is worth
a brief look. When Ford stated in the second presidential
debate that "there is no Soviet domination of Eastern Europe,"
the mass audience hardly noticed but reporters instantly saw
the remark as a gaffe. Polls showed that, citizens polled
immediately after the debate judged 44% to 33% that Ford had
won. Once the media frenzy of this famous gaffe had run its
course, however, the public's judgment was reversed: Several
days after the debate, the public thought by a margin of
62-17% that Carter had won. More significantly, Ford also lost
ground in straw poll surveys on how people intended to vote.
But by about 10 days later, Ford's poll standing had recovered
and the gaffe was left for political scientists to ponder
(Chaffee and Sears 1979; Sabato 1993, 127-29).
According to a study by Daron Shaw (1995), this pattern is
typical. Media frenzies over gaffes and alleged gaffes in
presidential campaigns do affect public support for the
candidates, but only briefly. The time it takes public opinion
to bounce back may, as in the Ford example, disrupt a
candidate's momentum and perhaps thereby affect the election,
but the lasting direct effect of most media frenzies tends to
be nil.
One way to think about this pattern is to assume that there
is some "natural" level of support for candidates that is
determined by political fundamentals such as the strength of
the economy, the candidates' positions on issues, and other
such matters. Media frenzies can briefly undermine a
candidate's natural level support, but cannot permanently
lower it. Thus, what happened to Clinton in the Lewinsky
matter is similar to what happens to candidates who misstep in
elections; he recovered from the initial attack. The fact that
Clinton gained back more support than he lost is harder to
explain in these terms, but I offer the following conjecture:
In non-electoral periods, the public tunes out from politics,
failing, inter alia, to keep its evaluation of presidential
performance fully up-to-date. But when, as in the early days
of the Lewinsky matter, Clinton's capacity to remain in office
came into question, the public took stock and reached a
conclusion that led to higher levels of overall support for
the threatened leader.
These observations suggest a rough generalization about
when media frenzies have lasting effects on opinion and when
they don't: The closer media frenzies get to what I am calling
political substance, the more likely the effects are to be
lasting. The example of Ford's pardon of Nixon would seem to
fit this pattern. To take one other example, it seems likely
that sympathetic press coverage of attacks by racist
southerners on peaceful civil rights protesters in the 1960s
had an important effect on northern opinion and thereby
congressional action. This was exactly what the Rev. Martin
Luther King Jr. expected to happen, and it had lasting
importance.
One lesson for political science from the Lewinsky poll
bounce, then, is that more attention needs to be given to the
general question of when Media Politics (in the sense of
trying to mobilize public support through mass communication)
matters and when it doesn't, and to do so in a manner that
doesn't presuppose the answer. A current research project of
Larry Bartels shows how this can be done: With a measure of
the "real economy" from the Commerce Department and a measure
of the "media economy" from content analysis of media
coverage, he hopes to find out which has more influence on
presidential approval. Among the auxiliary variables whose
impact on presidential approval he will assess is the
white-collar unemployment rate in Manhattan. The results will
be interesting however they come out.
Another lesson for political science from the Lewinsky poll
bounce is that the public is, within broad limits,
functionally indifferent to presidential character. "Don't
Ask, Don't Tell," as my colleague Art Stein summarizes the
mass attitude. Given this, it seems appropriate to consider
carefully whether research on the public's assessment of
presidential character really helps us to understand the
dynamics of American politics.
Contrary to this suggestion, it might be argued that
private sexual misbehavior is different from public character,
especially in light of changing sexual mores in this area, and
that voters' assessments of public character will remain
important. Perhaps. But if we view the character issue more
broadly, it seems unlikely that voter concern about character
has ever been very great. For example, Richard Nixon's
peculiar shortcomings were deeply felt by a large number of
voters from the moment he stepped onto the national stage in
the 1940s. Further, the concerns about Nixon's public
character were more serious than any that have been raised
about Clinton's. Yet Nixon was elected to the presidency
twice, once over Hubert Humphrey, a man whose sterling
character has been almost universally acknowledged. Nixon's
campaign against Humphrey was, of course, framed by urban
riots and a stalemated war in Asia, and in these
circumstances, Nixon chose to emphasize substance rather than
character. "When you're in trouble," he told voters, "you
don't turn to the men who got you in trouble to get you out of
it. I say we can't be led in the '70s by the men who stumbled
in the 60's."(10) Voters agreed with this emphasis, as they
almost always do.
Notes
Thanks to Larry Bartels, Dick Brody, Mo Fiorina, Fred
Greenstein, John Petrocik, and Daron Shaw for advice on early
drafts of this paper.
1. Documentation of the sources of polls cited in this
paper may be found in a PC Excel 5.0 file labeled "Lewpols" on
my web page (www.sscnet.ucla.edu/polisci/faculty/zaller). The polls used in determining
the overall effect of the Lewinsky matter are: ABC
News-Washington Post, January 19 and 31, job approval rates of
59% and 67%; ABC News, January 13 and 30, job approval of 62%
and 69%; CBS News, January 18 and February 1, 58% and 72%;
Newsweek, January 18 and 30, 61% and 70%; Time-CNN, January 15
and 31, 59% and 72%; U.S. News and World Report, January 11
and February 1, 58% and 66%. In cases in which polling
occurred over several days, the date given is for the final
day. Although wordings of the questions differ, all refer more
or less directly to Clinton's job performance rather than to
the Lewinsky matter per se.
2. Gallup conducted a poll on the afternoon of the first
day of the episode, prior to the evening news. This poll
showed Clinton's support rising to 62% from 60% two days
earlier. However, I do not count this poll on the grounds
that, although the story had broken at the time of the poll,
few Americans could yet have learned about it.
3. The sizes of the two surveys were 864 and 672.
4. Peter Jennings, quoting an anonymous politician.
5. The baseline for the ABC poll was actually January
25-26, with a sample of 1023. The size of the ABC postspeech
survey was 528. The NBC pre- and postspeech surveys both have
reported sizes of 405.
6. For sources of these and other favorability polls, see
the PC Excel file labeled "Lewpols" on my webpage
(www.sscnet.ucla.edu/polisci/faculty/zaller).
7. President Bush showed that short, successful wars that
cost few American lives do not harm popularity; but neither
are they much help over the longer run.
8. Full details of the ideological coding are available
upon request.
9. Though going beyond the direct evidence, this analysis
suggests that Clinton's confrontation with Congress over the
budget in early 1996, in which he reestablished his reputation
as a defender of centrist policies, may have been as important
to his November win as the economy.
10. Quoted in Newsweek, November 4, 1968, p. 28.
References
Alesina, Alberto, John Londregan, and Howard Rosenthal.
1993. "A Model of the Political Economy of the United States."
American Political Science Review 87(1):12-33.
Brody, Richard. 1991. Assessing the President: The Media,
Elite Opinion, and Public Support. Stanford: Stanford
University Press.
Chaffee, Steven, and David Sears. 1979. "Uses and Effects
of the 1976 Presidential Debates: An Overview of Empirical
Studies." In The Great Debates: Carter vs. Ford, ed. Sidney
Kraus. Bloomington: University of Indiana Press.
Connolly, Cecil, and Thomas B. Edsall. 1998. "Political
Pros Looking for Explanations: Public Reaction Seems to
Rewrite the Rules." Washington Post, February 9, A6.
Edelman, Murray. 1964. Symbolic Uses of Politics. Urbana:
University of Illinois Press.
-----. 1988. Constructing the Political Spectacle. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Fiorina, Morris. 1981. Retrospective Voting in American
National Elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Key, V. O. Jr. 1966. The Responsible Electorate:
Rationality in Presidential Voting, 1936-60. New York:
Vintage.
Kramer, Gerald H. 1971. "Short-Term Fluctuations in U.S.
Voting Behavior, 1896-1964." American Political Science Review
65(1):131-43.
Mueller, John. 1973. War, Presidents, and Public Opinion.
New York: Wiley.
Poole, Keith. Forthcoming. "Recovering a Basic Space from a
Set of Issue Scales." American Journal of Political Science 42
(August).
Rosenstone, Steven. 1983. Forecasting Presidential
Elections. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Sabato, Larry. 1993. Feeding Frenzy: How Attack Journalism
Has Transformed American Politics. New York: Free Press.
Shaw, Daron. 1995. "Strong Persuasion? The Effect of
Campaigns in U.S. Presidential Elections." Ph.D diss. UCLA.
Tulis, Jeffrey. 1987. The Rhetorical Presidency. Princeton:
Princeton University Press.
"TV Ratings from Speech." 1998. Los Angeles Times, January
29, A11.
Zaller, John. 1992. Nature and
Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
About the Author
John R. Zaller is coauthor (with
Herb McClosky) of American Ethos (1984) and author of Nature
and Origins of Mass Opinion (1992). He is completing a booked
called Theory of Media Politics: How the Interests of
Politicians, Journalists, and Citizens Shape the News
(University of Chicago Press). He received his Ph.D. from the
University of California, Berkeley in 1984. |